Anderson, M. L. (2010) “Neural re-use as a fundamental organizational principle of the brain”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 245–313.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000853
Ball, T., J. Derix, J. Wentlandt, B. Wieckhorst, O. Speck, A. Schulze-Bonhage, et al. (2009) “Anatomical specificity of functional amygdala imaging of responses to stimuli with positive and negative emotional valence”. Journal of Neuroscientific Methods 180, 1, 57–70.
doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.02.022
Bargh, J. A. and T. L. Chartrand (1999) “The unbearable automaticity of being”. American Psychologist 54, 7, 462–479.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462
Batson, C. D., B. D. Duncan, P. Ackerman, T. Buckley, and K. Birch (1981) “Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40, 2, 290–302.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.290
Berker, S. (2009) “The normative insignificance of neuroscience”. Philosophy and Public Affairs 37, 4, 293–329.
doi:10.1111/j.1088-4963.2009.01164.x
Botvinick, M. M., T. S. Braver, D. M. Barch, C. S. Carter, and J. D. Cohen (2001) “Conflict monitoring and cognitive control”. Psychological Review 108, 3, 624–652.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624
Buckholtz, J. W., C. L. Asplund, P. E. Dux, D. H. Zald, J. C. Gore, O. D. Jones, et al. (2008) “The neural correlates of third-party punishment”. Neuron 60, 5, 930–940.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.016
Bush, G. (1990) “Presidential Proclamation 6158”. Retrieved January 3, 2011, from http://www.loc.gov/loc/brain/proclaim.html
Carpendale, J. I. M. and D. L. Krebs, (1992) “Situational variation in moral judgment – in a stage or on a stage”. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 21, 2, 203–224.
doi:10.1007/BF01537337
Casebeer, W. D. (2003) “Moral cognition and its neural constituents”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 10, 840–846.
doi:10.1038/nrn1223
Casebeer, W. D. and P. S. Churchland (2003) “The neural mechanisms of moral cognition: a multiple-aspect approach to moral judgment and decision-making”. Biology and Philosophy 18, 1, 169–194.
doi:10.1023/A:1023380907603
Cavanna, A. E. and M. R. Trimble (2006) “The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates”. Brain 129, 3, 564–583.
doi:10.1093/brain/awl004
Chaiken, S. and Y. Trope (1999) Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Ciaramelli, E., M. Muccioli, E. Làdavas, and G. di Pellegrino (2007) “Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex”. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2, 84–92.
doi:10.1093/scan/nsm001
Crockett, M. J., L. Clark, M. D. Hauser, and T. W. Robbins (2010) “Serotonin selectively influences moral judgment and behavior through effects on harm aversion”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 40, 17433–17438.
doi:10.1073/pnas.1009396107
Ekstrom, A. (2010) “How and when the fMRI BOLD signal relates to underlying neural activity: the danger in dissociation”. Brain Research Review 62, 2, 233–244.
doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.12.004
Farah, M. J. (2010) Neuroethics: an introduction with readings. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Freud, S. (1930) Das Unbehagen in der Kultur. Wien: Internationaler Psychoanalytischer Verlag.
Friston, K. J. (2009) “Modalities, modes, and models in functional neuroimaging”. Science 326, 5951, 399–403.
doi:10.1126/science.1174521
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005) The ethical brain. New York and Washington, D.C.: DANA Press.
Giordano, J. J. and B. Gordijn (2010) Scientific and philosophical perspectives in neuroethics. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511676505
Glenn, A. L., A. Raine, and R. A. Schug (2009) “The neural correlates of moral decision-making in psychopathy”. Molecular Psychiatry 14, 1, 5–6.
doi:10.1038/mp.2008.104
Greene, J. and J. Haidt (2002) “How (and where) does moral judgment work?”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 12, 517–523.
doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02011-9
Greene, J. D. (2007) “The secret joke of Kant’s soul”. In Moral psychology. The neuroscience of morality: emotion, brain disorders, and development. Vol. 3, 35–79. W. Sinnott-Armstrong, ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Greene, J. D., S. A. Morelli, K. Lowenberg, L. E. Nvstrom, and J. D. Cohen (2008) “Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment”. Cognition 107, 3, 1144–1154.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004
Greene, J. D., L. E. Nystrom, A. D. Engell, J. M. Darley, and J. D. Cohen (2004) “The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment”. Neuron 44, 2, 389–400.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027
Greene, J. D., R. B. Sommerville, L. E. Nystrom, J. M. Darley, and J. D. Cohen (2001) “An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment”. Science 293, 5537, 2105–2108.
doi:10.1126/science.1062872
Haidt, J. (2001) “The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment”. Psychological Review 108, 4, 814–834.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814
Haidt, J. (2007) “The new synthesis in moral psychology”. Science 316, 5827, 998–1002.
doi:10.1126/science.1137651
Haidt, J. and S. Kesebir (2010) “Morality”. In Handbook of Social Psychology. 5th ed., 797–832. S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, and G. Lindzey, eds. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Haidt, J., S. H. Koller, and M. G. Dias (1993) “Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65, 4, 613–628.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.613
Haidt, J., P. Rozin, C. R. McCauley, and S. Imada (1997) “Body, psyche, and culture: the relationship between disgust and morality”. Psychology and Developing Societies 9, 107–131.
doi:10.1177/097133369700900105
Hauser, M. D. (2006) “The liver and the moral organ”. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences 1, 3, 214–220.
doi:10.1093/scan/nsl026
Heeger, D. J. and D. Ress (2002) “What does fMRI tell us about neuronal activity?”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, 2, 142–151.
doi:10.1038/nrn730
Heekeren, H. R., I. Wartenburger, H. Schmidt, K. Prehn, H. P. Schwintowski, and A. Villringer (2005) “Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making”. Neuroimage 24, 3, 887–897.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.026
Heekeren, H. R., I. Wartenburger, H. Schmidt, H. P. Schwintowski, and A. Villringer (2003) “An fMRI study of simple ethical decision-making”. Neuroreport 14, 9, 1215–1219.
doi:10.1097/00001756-200307010-00005
Helmuth, L. (2001) “Cognitive neuroscience – moral reasoning relies on emotion”. Science 293, 5537, 1971–1972.
doi:10.1126/science.293.5537.1971a
Huebner, B., S. Dwyer, and M. Hauser (2009) “The role of emotion in moral psychology”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 1, 1–6.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.006
Illes, J. (2006) Neuroethics: defining the issues in theory, practice, and policy. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.
Kahane, G. and N. Shackel (2008) “Do abnormal responses show utilitarian bias?” Nature 452, E5.
doi:10.1038/nature06785
Kahane, G. and N. Shackel (2010) “Methodological issues in the neuroscience of moral judgement”. Mind and Language 25, 5, 561–582.
doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2010.01401.x
Kamm, F. M. (2009) “Neuroscience and moral reasoning: a note on recent research”. Philosophy and Public Affairs 37, 4, 330–345.
doi:10.1111/j.1088-4963.2009.01165.x
Killgore, W. D. S., D. B. Killgore, L. M. Day, C. Li, G. H. Kamimori, and T. J. Balkin (2007) “The effects of 53 hours of sleep deprivation on moral judgment”. Sleep 30, 3, 345–352.
Koechlin, E., G. Basso, P. Pietrini, S. Panzer, and J. Grafman (1999) “The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition”. Nature 399, 6732, 148–151.
doi:10.1038/20178
Koenigs, M., L. Young, R. Adolphs, D. Tranel, F. Cushman, M. Hauser, et al. (2007) “Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements”. Nature 446, 7138, 908–911.
doi:10.1038/nature05631
Kohlberg, L. and B. Puka (1994) Kohlberg’s original study of moral development. New York: Garland.
Levy, N. (2007) Neuroethics. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.
Logothetis, N. K. (2008) “What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI”. Nature 453, 7197, 869–878.
doi:10.1038/nature06976
Logothetis, N. K., J. Pauls, M. Augath, T. Trinath, and A. Oeltermann (2001) “Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal”. Nature 412, 6843, 150–157.
doi:10.1038/35084005
Logothetis, N. K. and B. A.Wandell (2004) “Interpreting the BOLD signal”. Annual Review of Physiology 66, 735–769.
doi:10.1146/annurev.physiol.66.082602.092845
McCabe, D. P. and A. D. Castel (2008) “Seeing is believing: the effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning”. Cognition 107, 1, 343–352.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.017
McGuire, J., R. Langdon, M. Coltheart, and C. Mackenzie (2009) “A reanalysis of the personal/ impersonal distinction in moral psychology research”. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45, 3, 577–580.
doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.01.002
Moll, J. and R. de Oliveira-Souza (2007) “Moral judgments, emotions and the utilitarian brain”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11, 8, 319–321.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.06.001
Moll, J., R. de Oliveira-Souza, I. E. Bramati, and J. Grafman (2002a) “Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments”. Neuroimage 16, 3, 696–703.
doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1118
Moll, J., R. de Oliveira-Souza, P. J. Eslinger, I. E. Bramati, J. Mourao-Miranda, P. A. Andreiuolo, et al. (2002b) “The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions”. Journal of Neuroscience 22, 7, 2730–2736.
Moll, J., R. de Oliveira-Souza, F. T. Moll, F. A. Ignacio, I. E. Bramati, E. M. Caparelli-Daquer, et al. (2005b) “The moral affiliations of disgust – a functional MRI study”. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 18, 1, 68–78.
doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7
Moll, J., P. J. Eslinger, and R. de Oliveira-Souza (2001) “Frontopolar and anterior temporal cortex activation in a moral judgment task – preliminary functional MRI results in normal subjects”. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria 59, 3B, 657–664.
doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2001000500001
Moll, J., F. Krueger, R. Zahn, M. Pardini, R. de Oliveira-Souzat, and J. Grafman (2006) “Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, 42, 15623–15628.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0604475103
Moll, J., R. Zahn, R. de Oliveira-Souza, F. Krueger, and J. Grafman (2005a) “The neural basis of human moral cognition”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6, 10, 799–809.
doi:10.1038/nrn1768
Monin, B., D. A. Pizarro, and J. S. Beer (2007) “Reason and emotion in moral judgment: different prototypes lead to different theories”. In Do emotions help or hurt decision making? A hedgefoxian perspective, 219–244. K. D. Vohs, R. F. Baumeister, and G. Loewenstein, eds. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Moore, A. B., B. A. Clark, and M. J. Kane (2008) “Who shall not kill? Individual differences in working memory capacity, executive control, and moral judgment”. Psychological Science 19, 6, 549–557.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02122.x
Moore, G. E. (1903) Principia ethica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Morse, S. J. (2006) “Brain overclaim syndrome and criminal responsibility: a diagnostic note”. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law 3, 397–412.
Mosso, A. (1881) Ueber den Kreislauf des Blutes im Menschlichen Gehirn. Untersuchungen. Leipzig.
Nadelhoffer, T., E. A. Nahmias, and S. Nichols (2010) Moral psychology: historical and contemporary readings. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Nagel, S. K. (2010) Ethics and the neurosciences: ethical and social consequences of neuroscientific progress. Paderborn: Mentis.
Nisbett, R. E. and T. D. Wilson (1977) “Telling more than we can know – verbal reports on mental processes”. Psychological Review 84, 3 231–259.
Piaget, J. (1932) Le jugement moral chez l’enfant. Paris: Librairie Fèlix Alcan.
Poldrack, R. A. (2006) “Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data?”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10, 2, 59–63.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004
Prehn, K., I. Wartenburger, K. Meriau, C. Scheibe, O. R. Goodenough, A. Villringer, et al. (2008) “Individual differences in moral judgment competence influence neural correlates of socio-normative judgments”. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3, 1, 33–46.
doi:10.1093/scan/nsm037
Racine, E. (2010) Pragmatic neuroethics: improving treatment and understanding of the mind-brain. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
Racine, E., O. Bar-Ilan, and J. Illes (2005) “fMRI in the public eye”. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6, 2, 159–164.
doi:10.1038/nrn1609
Racine, E., O. Bar-Ilan, and J. Illes (2006) “Brain imaging – a decade of coverage in the print media”. Science Communication 28, 1, 122–143.
doi:10.1177/1075547006291990
Raichle, M. E. and M. A. Mintun (2006) “Brain work and brain imaging”. Annual Review of Neuroscience 29, 449–476.
doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112819
Roskies, A. (2002) “Neuroethics for the new millennium”. Neuron 35, 1, 21–23.
doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00763-8
Schleim, S. (2008b) “Moral physiology, its limitations and philosophical implications”. Jahrbuch für Wissenschaft und Ethik 13, 51–80.
Schleim, S. (2011) Die Neurogesellschaft: Wie die Hirnforschung Recht und Moral herausfordert. Hannover: Heise Verlag.
Schleim, S. and J. P. Roiser (2009) “fMRI in translation: the challenges facing real-world applications”. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3, 63, 1–7.
Schleim, S., T. M. Spranger, S. Erk, and H. Walter (2011) “From moral to legal judgment: the influence of normative context in lawyers and other academics”. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6, 48–57.
doi:10.1093/scan/nsq010
Schnall, S., J. Haidt, G. L. Clore, and A. H. Jordan (2008) “Disgust as embodied moral judgment”. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34, 8, 1096–1109.
doi:10.1177/0146167208317771
Schummers, J., H. B.Yu, and M. Sur (2008) “Tuned responses of astrocytes and their influence on hemodynamic signals in the visual cortex”. Science 320, 5883, 1638–1643.
doi:10.1126/science.1156120
Sidgwick, H. (1907) The method of ethics. 7th ed. London: Macmillan and Co.
Singer, P. (2005) “Ethics and intuitions”. Journal of Ethics 9, 331–352.
doi:10.1007/s10892-005-3508-y
Sirotin, Y. B. and A. Das (2009) “Anticipatory haemodynamic signals in sensory cortex not predicted by local neuronal activity”. Nature 457, 7228, 475–476.
doi:10.1038/nature07664
Sperry, R. W. (1981a) “Nobel lecture: some effects of disconnecting the cerebral hemispheres”. Retrieved January 3 2011, from http://nobelprize.org/nobel_prizes/medicine/laureates/1981/ sperry-lecture.html
Sperry, R. W. (1981b) “Changing priorities”. Annual Review of Neuroscience 4, 1–15.
doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.04.030181.000245
Thomson, J. J. (1985) “The Trolley problem”. The Yale Law Journal 94, 6, 1395–1415.
doi:10.2307/796133
Turiel, E. (2010) “Snap judgment? Not so fast: thought, reasoning, and choice as psychological realities”. Human Development 53, 3, 105–109.
doi:10.1159/000315167
Unger, P. (1996) Living high and letting die: our illusion of innocence. New York: Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1093/0195108590.001.0001
Weber, F. and H. Knopf (2006) “Incidental findings in magnetic resonance imaging of the brains of healthy young men”. Journal of Neurological Sciences 240, 1–2, 81–84.
doi:10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.008
Weisberg, D. S., F. C. Keil, J. Goodstein, E. Rawson, and J. R. Gray (2008) “The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations”. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences 20, 3, 470–477.
doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20040
Wheatley, T. and J. Haidt (2005) “Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe”. Psychological Science 16, 10, 780–784.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01614.x
Young, L., F. Cushman, M. Hauser, and R. Saxe (2007) “The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, 20, 8235–8240.
doi:10.1073/pnas.0701408104
doi:10.1038/nrn2776