ESTONIAN ACADEMY
PUBLISHERS
eesti teaduste
akadeemia kirjastus
PUBLISHED
SINCE 1997
 
TRAMES cover
TRAMES. A Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences
ISSN 1736-7514 (Electronic)
ISSN 1406-0922 (Print)
Impact Factor (2022): 0.2
MOTHERS’ CONVERSATIONAL STYLE IN A CHANGING DEVELOPMENTAL CONTEXT; pp. 277–286
PDF | https://doi.org/10.3176/tr.2019.3.01

Author
Tiia Tulviste
Abstract

The societal changes in Estonia have altered the beliefs and values related to childrearing in the direction of stressing more autonomy and self-direction in children. This study addresses the question if and to what extent the amount of talk and communicative intents of Estonian mothers have changed over time period of approximately 25 years. With this aim, we compared mothers’ interactions with two-year-old children in years 1992, 2000, and 2017. The results show that mothers have become less directive during puzzle solving but not at meals. Mothers’ interaction with children has not changed in respect of the amount of talk produced by mothers and children. Moreover, the study demonstrated that mothers’ conversational style, as well as changes in it over time, depends largely on interactional context.

References

Bornstein, Marc H., Joseph Tal, and Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda (1991) “Parenting in cross-cultural perspective: the United States, France, and Japan”. In Marc H. Bornstein, ed. Cultural approaches to parenting, 69-90. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Chapman, Robin S. (2000) “Children’s language learning: an interactionist perspective”. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 41, 33–54.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021963099004953
https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00548

Demuth, Carolin (2015) “Mother-child communication: cultural differences”. In James D. Wright, ed. International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences. Vol.15, 874–880. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23209-1

Fernald, Anne, Virginia A. Marchman, and Adriana Weisleder (2013) “SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months”. Developmental Science 16, 234–248.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019

Halle, Tamara and Marilyn Shatz (1994) “Mothers’ social regulatory language to young children in family settings”. First Language 14, 83–104.
https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379401404006

Hart, Betty and Todd R. Risley (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

Heath, Shirley B. (1983) Ways with words: language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511841057

Hoff, Erika (2006) “How social contexts support and shape language development”. Developmental Review 26, 55–88.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002

Hoff, Erika, Brett Laursen, and Twila Tardif (2002) “Socioeconomic status and parenting”. In Marc H. Bornstein, ed. Handbook of parenting, 231–252. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika (1991) “Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings”. Child Development 62, 782–796.
https://doi.org/10.2307/1131177

Huttenlocher, Janellen, Wendy Haight, Anthony Bryk, Michael Seltzer, and Thomas Lyons (1991) “Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender”. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236

Inglehart, Ronald, Christian Haerpfer, Alejandro Moreno, Christian Welzel, Kseniya Kizilova, Jaime Diez-Medrano, Marta Lagos, Pippa Norris, Eduard Ponarin, Bi Puranen, et al., eds. (2014)World values survey: round six – country-pooled datafile version. Madrid: JD Systems Institute.

www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV6.jsp

Junefelt, Karin and Tiia Tulviste (1997) “Regulation and praise in American, Estonian, and Swedish mother-child interaction”. Mind, Culture, and Activity: An International Journal 4, 24–33.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327884mca0401_3

Kagitçibaşi, Çiğdem (2005) “Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: implications for self and family”. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 36, 403–422.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022105275959

McDonald, Lynda and Diana Pien (1982) “Mother conversational behavior as a function of interactional intent”. Journal of Child Language 9, 337–58.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000475X

Masur, Elise Frank, Valerie Flynn, and Doreen L. Eichorst (2005) “Maternal responsive and directive behaviors and utterances as predictors of children’s lexical development”. Journal of Child Language 32, 63–91.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006634

Schieffelin, Bambi B. and Elinor Ochs, eds. (1986) Language socialization across cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620898

Rowe, Meredith L. (2012) “A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development”. Child Development 83, 1762–1774.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x

Snow, Catherine E., and Charles A. Ferguson, eds. (1977) Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Tulviste, Tiia (2003) “Contextual variability in interactions between mothers and 2-year-olds”. First Language 23, 311–325.
https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237030233004

Tulviste, Tiia (2004) “Socio-cultural variation in mothers’ control over children’s behavior”. Ethos 32, 34–50.
https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.2004.32.1.34

Tulviste, Tiia and Maaret Ahtonen (2007) “Child-rearing values of Estonian and Finnish mothers and fathers”. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 38, 137–155.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022106297297

Tulviste, Tiia and Kenn Konstabel (2017) “People’s age affects what qualities they value in children: a comparison of ethnic Estonians and Russian-speaking minority across time”. Trames, 21, 4, 299–312.
https://doi.org/10.3176/tr.2017.4.01

Tulviste, Tiia and Margit Raudsepp (1997) “The conversational style of Estonian mothers”. First Language 17, 151–163.
https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379701705107
https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379701705002

Tulviste, Tiia, Luule Mizera, and Boel De Geer (2012) “Socialization values in stable and changing societies: a comparative study of Estonian, Swedish, and Russian Estonian mothers”. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 43, 480–497.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111401393

Tulviste, Tiia, Luule Mizera, Boel De Geer, and Marja-Terttu Tryggvason (2003) “A silent Finn, a silent Finno-Ugric, or a silent Nordic? A comparative study of Estonian, Finnish and Swedish mother-adolescent interactions”. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 249–265.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000146

Tulviste, Tiia, Pirko Tõugu, Heidi Keller, Lisa Schröder, and Boel De Geer (2016) “Children’s and mothers’ contribution to joint reminiscing in different sociocultural contexts: who speaks and what is said”. Infant and Child Development 25, 43–63.
https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1921

Vibbert, Martha and Marc H. Bornstein, (1989) “Specific associations between domains of mother–child interaction and toddler referential language and pretense play”. Infant Behavior and Development 12, 163–184.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(89)90004-0

Weizman, Zehava Oz and Catherine E. Snow (2001) “Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning”. Developmental Psychology 37, 265–279. 
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.265

Yont, Kristine M., Catherine E Snow, and Lynne Vernon-Feagans (2003) “The role of context in mother–child interactions: an analysis of communicative intents expressed during toy play and book reading with 12-month-olds”. Journal of Pragmatics 35, 435–454.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(02)00144-3

Zhou Chan, Wai Ying Vivien Yiu, Michael Shengtao Wu, and Patricia M. Greenfield (2018) Perception of cross-generational differences in child behavior and parent socialization: a mixed-method interview study with grandmothers in China”. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 49, 62–81
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117736029

Back to Issue